Unami language

Unami
Native to Eastern United States
Region Around the lower Delaware and Hudson rivers in the United States; some Unami groups in Oklahoma
Extinct 2002[1]
Language codes
ISO 639-3 unm
Glottolog unam1242[2]

Map showing the aboriginal boundaries of Delaware territories, with Munsee territory and Unami dialectal divisions indicated. The territory of the poorly known Unalachtigo dialect of Unami is not clearly indicated, but is presumed to be approximately in the area of "Sankhikan" on the map.

Unami is an Algonquian language spoken by Lenape people in what was then the lower Hudson Valley area and New York Harbor area, New Jersey, Pennsylvania and Delaware, but later in Ontario and Oklahoma. It is one of the two Delaware languages, the other being Munsee. The last fluent speaker in the United States, Edward Thompson, of the Delaware Tribe of Indians, died on 31 August 2002.[1] His sister Nora Thompson Dean (1907–1984) provided valuable information about the language to linguists and other scholars.

Lenape is from /lənaːpːe/, a word in the Unami dialect whose most literal translation into English would be "common person".[3][4] The Lenape names for the areas they inhabited were Scheyichbi (i.e. New Jersey), which means "water's edge", and Lenapehoking, meaning "in the land of the Delaware Indians", although the latter is a term coined by the Unami speaker Nora Thompson Dean in 1984, to describe the ancient homeland of all Delaware Indians, both Unami and Munsee.[5][6] The English named the river running through much of the traditional range of the Lenape after the first governor of the Jamestown Colony, Lord De La Warr, and consequently referred to the people who lived around the river as "Delaware Indians".

History

Unami is an Eastern Algonquian language. The hypothetical common ancestor language from which the Eastern Algonquian languages descend is Proto-Eastern Algonquian (PEA). An intermediate group Delawarean that is a descendant of Proto-Eastern Algonquian consists of Mahican and Common Delaware, the latter being a further subgroup comprising Munsee Delaware and Unami Delaware.[7] The justification for Delawarean as an intermediate subgroup rests upon the high degree of similarity between Mahican and the two Delaware languages, but relatively little detailed argumentation in support of Delawarean has been adduced.[7][8][9] The International Encyclopedia of Linguistics lists Tla Wilano as one of the Delaware or Unami related languages. Until recently, Tla Wilano was spoken by members of the Ani-Stohini/Unami Nation in Virginia. Now there is only one speaker.

Compared to Munsee, Unami has undergone extensive phonological innovation, coupled with morphological regularization.[10]

The PEA vowel system consisted of four long vowels *i·, *o·, *e·, *a·, and two short vowels *a and ə. The vowel history is as follows: *i· (from PEA merger of Proto-Algonquian (PA *i· and *i to PEA *i·), *o· (from PEA merger of PA *o· and *o ), *e· (from Proto-Algonquian *e·), and *a· (from Proto-Algonquian *a·; the short vowels are (from Proto-Algonquian *e), and *a (from Proto-Algonquian *a). This system was continued down to Common Delaware,[11] but Munsee and Unami have innovated separately with respect to the vowel systems.

Revival attempts

The dominant modern version of the Southern Unami dialect called "Lenape" is taught by the Delaware Tribe of Indians, headquartered in Bartlesville, Oklahoma. They have the Lenape Language Preservation Project.[12] The same dialect was spoken by the Delaware Nation in Anadarko in the southwestern part of Oklahoma. Both Oklahoma Delaware tribes have recorded native speakers and produced written lessons for instruction, which are available for sale. These efforts, in conjunction with other community efforts comprise an attempt to preserve the language.

Some descriptions of the Northern Unami dialect as spoken during the 18th century are given by Moravian missionary John Heckewelder.[13]

Phonology

Consonants

Unami has been analyzed as having contrastive geminate and non-geminate obstruent consonants, although this contrast is relatively weak.[14] A full analysis of the status of the geminates, also known as long consonants, is not available, and more than one analysis of Delaware consonants has been proposed. The long consonants are described as having low functional yield, that is they differentiate relatively few pairs of words but do occur in contrasting environments.[15] Some examples of contrastive geminate pairs include: k·ə́ntka·n "then you (sg.) danced" versus kə́ntka·n "then there was dancing"; p·ɔ́·m "his thigh" versus pɔ́·m "the ham"; and nsa·s·a·k·ənə́mən "I stuck it out repeatedly" versus nsa·sa·k·ənə́mən "I stuck it out slowly". There are also rules that lengthen consonants in certain environments.[16] The length mark (ː) is used to indicate gemination of a preceding consonant or vowel length, although in the literature on Unami the raised dot (·) is often used for these purposes, as other diacritics may be used above vowels (see below).[17]

In the following chart, the usual transcription used in the sources is given with the IPA in brackets.

Consonants
Bilabial Dental Postalveolar Velar Glottal
Plosive p [p] t [t̪] č [tʃ] k [k]
Fricative s [s] š [ʃ] x [x] h [h]
Nasal m [m] n [n]
Lateral l [l]
Semivowel w [w] j [j]

Vowels

Unami vowels are presented as organized into contrasting long–short pairs.[18] One asymmetry is that high short /u/ is paired with long /oː/, and the pairing of long and short /ə/ is noteworthy. It is worth noting that /ə/ and /o/ are not distinguishable before /w/, /m/, and /kw/. Additionally, vowels are classified as strong and weak, which plays an important role in determining stress (see below). Long vowels and vowels before consonant clusters are automatically strong. Certain short vowels, which are differentiated with a breve – /ǐ, ě, ǒ/ – are also strong vowels because they are treated morphophonemically as long vowels, even though they are pronounced as short. In a sequence of syllables containing a short vowel followed by a consonant (C) or consonant and /w/ (Cw), the odd-numbered vowels are weak, and the even numbered vowels are strong. Furthermore, some short vowels are strong even in a weakening environment; these exceptions are often marked with a grave accent. Additionally, some vowels which are unaffected by predicted vowel syncope are marked with an acute accent.[19] There is a predictable tendency, additionally, to nasalize and lengthen a vowel before /ns/ and /nš/, so that /lowé·nso/ ("his name is [such]") is realized closer to [luwé̹·su] from underlying /ələwe·nsəw/.[20]

Long vowels
Front Central Back
Close
Mid əː ɔː
Open
Short vowels
Front Central Back
Close i u
Mid e ə ɔ
Open a

Syllable structure

Syllable structure is diverse, permitting a certain amount of consonant clustering. The following consonant clusters can occur:[21]

Additionally, certain consonants may combine with the semivowel /w/. Note that some underlying forms may also contain /sw/ and /šw/, but these are always removed by morphophonemic processes.[22]

Stress

Stress is generally predictable in Unami. The rightmost nonfinal strong vowel is stressed, or a strong vowel in final position if it is the only one in the word. Often when stress would be expected to fall on the antepenult it is shifted to the penult. This change is found in three conjunct endings: /-ak/, /-at/, and /-an/. In the last case, the accent shifts to the penultimate /-an/ only if it would otherwise fall on an antepenultimate short vowel, and if the consonant between them is voiced.[23]

Phonological processes

Unami phonology is extremely complex, with various morphophonological rules, which is to say, a theoretical form will undergo a set of predictable phonological processes to produce the true form found in speech (there are some exceptions to these rules, however.) There are about 17 such rules common to both Munsee and Unami, and another 28 unique to Unami, though this analysis ignores predictable exceptions, such as the class of static words which may skip many of these rules. These rules govern things such as consonant lengthening/shortening, vowel syncopation, metathesis, vowel coloring, etc.[24]

A list of processes unique to Unami follow.[25] Note that these are written in linguistic notation. Thus, {ə,a} → ∅ / _{h, x}V when {ə,a} are weak means that the sounds /ə/ and /a/ become null (disappear) in the context of when they are weak and appear before either /h/ or /x/ and another vowel. The slash means "in the context of", and the underscore _ indicates where the /ə/ or /a/ must occur. In some notations the pound symbol (#) appears, indicating word boundaries (either the beginning or end). Regular parenthesis indicate optional conditions when framing phonemes or additional information about phonemes (e.g., "C=stop"). The capital letters C, V, and N mean "consonant", "vowel", and "nasal" respectively.

Morphology

Nouns

Third person participants are marked for gender (animate versus inanimate), obviation (proximate versus obviative), and presence (nonabsentative versus absentative). Generally, the inanimate, obviative, and absentative categories are more marked than their opposites (i.e. animate, proximate, and nonabsentative), however it is not clear whether animacy or inanimacy is the more marked of the opposition. The first and second persons are not marked for presence or obviation, and are always animate.[26]

Obviation

The first mentioned and/or primary animate third person is proximate; all other third persons are obviative, unless they act in conjunction with the proximate participant. Verbs are also inflected to indicate whether the verbal action is proximate on obviate or obviate on proximate.

Presence

Third person participants can be marked with a special set of endings indicating their absence from the general area of the focus of discourse. For example, absentative endings are used when speaking of the deceased (even if the corpse is physically present), as in the sentence no·lăčahko·ná·na nkahe·səná·na ("our (excl.) mother (abv.) treated us well"), in which both verb and noun are marked with the absentative /-a/ ending.

Gender

Nouns in Unami are classified as either animate or inanimate, and this is reflected in verbal conjugation. Animate nouns denote human beings, animals, spirits, and trees, as well as certain fruits, tubers, root vegetables and other unpredictable exceptions like /ko·n/ ("snow") and /nhíkaš/ ("my fingernail"). (Note, however, that berries, nuts, and vegetables growing above ground are generally inanimate.) Thus, /té·hi·m/ ("strawberry"), /xáskwi·m/ ("corn"), /ke·skúnthak/ ("pumpkin"), /mpi/ ("water"), and /nhíka·t/ ("my leg") are inanimate, while /lə́nu/ ("man"), /xho·k/ ("snake"), /mahtán'tu/ ("Devil") and /hɔ́pəni·s/ ("potato") are animate. However, traditionally inanimate nouns which are directly addressed or personified are treated as animate. Thus, traditionally inanimate /ăsǝ́n/ ("stone") is treated as animate in the sentence šá·i a· ăsǝ́nak kǝnčí·mowak ("the stones would immediately cry out").[27]

Verbs

Unami is a highly agglutinative, polysynthetic language. Verbs in Unami are marked for person and number, and contain inflectional elements of order (independent, conjunct, and imperative), aspect, and the negative.[28]

A table of the personal pronouns is given below. Note that the first person plural ("we") may be either inclusive - that is, including the addressee, - or exclusive.

Personal Pronouns[29]
Person Singular Plural
First ni· ni·ló·na (exclusive) ki·ló·na (inclusive)
Second ki· ki·ló·wa
Third né·k·a ne·k·a·ɔ
Indefinite ∅ (zero ending) ∅ (zero ending)

Following are tables exemplifying verbal paradigms in Unami in the independent order, indicative mood and present tense.[30]

Animate Intransitive (AI) Verbs

Animate Intransitive Independent Indicative
Person /kəntəka·-/
'dance'
/wəm-/
'come from'
1,2 nkə́ntka
/n-,k--/
no·m
/n-,k--/
3 kə́ntke·(w)
/-w/
wəm
/-w/
1p,12 nkəntkáhəna
/n-,k--hməna·/
no·mhóməna
/n-,k--hməna·/
2p kkəntkáhəmɔ
/k--hmwa·/
ko·mhómɔ
/k--hmwa·/
3p kəntké·yɔk
/-wak/
mo·k
/-wak/
X kə́ntkan
/-n/
xahé·lən
/-n/

Transitive Animate (TA) Verbs

Transitive Animate Independent Indicative, Theme 1
Objective /mi·l-/
'give to'
Absolute /lo·sw/
'burn'
Person 3 3p -
1,2 nǝmí·la
/n-,k--a·/
nǝmi·lá·ɔk
/n-,k--a·wak/
nló·sa
/n-,k--a·/
3 mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
mwi·lá·ɔ
/w--a·wa(l)/
ló·se·(w)
/-e·w/
1p,12 nǝmi·lá·wǝna
/n-,k--a·wǝna·/
nǝmi·la·wǝná·na(·)k
/n-,k--a·wǝnă̆·k/
nlo·sáhǝna
/n-,k--a·hmǝna·/
2p kǝmi·láwwa
/k--a·wǝwa·/
kǝmi·láwwa·ɔk
/k--a·wǝwa·wak/
kǝlo·sáhǝmɔ
/k--a·hmwa·/
3p mwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
mwi·lawwá·ɔ
/w--a·wǝwa·wa(l)/
lo·sé·yɔk
/-ǝ·wak/
X mí·la·(w)
/a·w/
mí·l·á·ɔk
/a·wak/
N/A

Inanimate Intransitive (II) Verbs

Inanimate Intransitive Independent Indicative
Person /màxhe·-/
'be red'
/alǝt-/
'rot'
IN máxke·(w)
/-w/
alǝ́t
/-w/
IN pl. maxké·yɔ(l)
/-wa(l)/
alǝ́t·o(·l)
/-wa(l)/

Transitive Inanimate (TI) Verbs

The TI themes have the same inflection as AI stems for all conjuncts (NB that indefinite subject forms of consonant-final themes are not attested, but the vowel-final themes follow the AI pattern.) Three forms are illustrated from each type.

Transitive Inanimate Independent Indicative
Type Subjects
2 3 3pl
1a pǝnáman
/-aman/
pǝnánk
/-ank/
pǝnamhíti·t
/-amǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
1b ló·sǝman
/-ǝman/
ló·sink
/-ǝnk/
lo·sǝmíhti·t
/-ǝmǝh(ǝ)ti·t/
2 wǝlí·taɔn
/-awan/
wǝlí·ta·kw
/-a·kw/
wǝli·tóhti·t
/-o·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-C/) né·man
/-an/
nenk
/-ǝk/
ne·mhíti·t
/-ǝ·h(ǝ)ti·t/
3(/-V) mí·č·iyan
/-yan/
mí·č·i·t
/-t/
mi·č·íhti·t
/-h(ǝ)ti·t/

Affixes

Prefixes

Verbal prefixes are used only in the independent order, although some forms of the independent order lack a prefix. There are three of them: /n-/ (first person), /k-/ (second person), and /w-/ (third person). If a stem has an underlying initial vowel, a /t/ is inserted after the prefix, and before this and other stem-initial consonants a /ə/ is inserted. Sometimes, this /ə/ contracts with a stem-initial /wə/ to /o·/ except when the /o·/ would be phonetically shortened via rule U-4a. Examples include: /ntá·mwi/ ("I get up from lying") versus /á·mwi·(w)/ ("he gets up"). Two roots with initial /t/ extend the syllable with /-ən/ when adding prefixes; these roots are /tal-/ ("there") and /tax-/ ("so many"), e.g. náni ntəntala·wsí·ne·n ("that is where we live [our lives]") from the animate intransitive stem /tala·wəsi·/.[31]

Prefixes are mutually exclusive and are selected based on the following rule: if one of the participants is second person, the second person prefix is used; if not, if one of the participants is the first person, then the first person prefix is used; if none of these applies, other forms, if they take a prefix, take the third person prefix. This is the well-known Algonquian 2-1-3 precedence rule.[32]

Suffixes

Suffixes are grouped into eight positional classes. These are:[31]

The first position (theme signs) is filled only for transitive verbs - these help describe the relationship between the two participants, indicating which is the agent and which is the object. The direct and inverse theme signs indicate the direction of the verb along a spectrum what might be called distance. From least to most distant the participants are: (1) first or second; (2) indefinite (only as subject); (3) proximate third person; (4) obviative third person; (5) farther obviative third person; (6) inanimate (subject only). If the subject is less distant than the object, the direct theme is used; if the subject is more distant, the inverse signs are used. After transitive animate (TA) verb stems appear one of the four following theme signs:

For transitive inanimate (TI) verbs there appears the theme sign pertaining to the appropriate TI class:

NB that for Class 2 TI theme signs, in certain derivational categories, the theme sign is retained even when the thematic element is dropped. The contrast between the two categories is sharper in the Central Algonquian languages, in which the theme sign has a more complex series of alternants.[33]

The second position consists of diminutives in ₂/-tī/ and pejoratives in ₂/-šī/ (in both of these, the /-ī/ is unstable.) An example of a diminutive is /pé·t·o/ ("the little one comes"), contrasted with the pejorative /pé·šo/ ("the undesirable one came"). The use of /t/ in the formation of diminutives seems to be an innovation of Unami, as many other Algonquian languages use */s/ or */ʃ/ (in fact, the diminutive of Munsee is /-šī/).

In position three are the affixes ₃/-lī/, which marks the obviative third person, and ₃/-h(ə)tī/, which marks the plural.

Position four contains the negative affix ₄/-(ō)w(ī)/ and prohibitive imperative and future imperative forms, which have complex series of alternants.

Position five contains the central endings which index the central participant of each form, except those using TA theme signs ₁/-i·/ and ₁/-əl/;

Position six contains the affix endings: /-pan/ marks the preterite, and /sa/ ~ /shan/ mark the present.

Position seven contains peripheral endings, which are used to mark the nominal category of some 3rd person participants in forms in the independent and conjunct (but not imperative) orders.

Position eight reflects the subjunctive, prohibitive, and future modes.

Syntax

Unami is, like many Algonquian languages, polysynthetic and highly agglutinative. This means that most of the information is encoded in the verb (sometimes with whole words being incorporated into the stem), making word order more fluid than in English. Unfortunately, syntax is one of the least studied aspects of the Unami language; there is much more data on morphology, because of an especial focus on reconstructing Proto-Algonquian.

Some examples of complex sentences in Unami include:[34]

Notes

  1. 1 2 Chambers, Steve. "The vanishing voice of the Lenape." Star-Ledger. 17 Nov 2002. Retrieved 8 Dec 2013.
  2. Hammarström, Harald; Forkel, Robert; Haspelmath, Martin; Bank, Sebastian, eds. (2016). "Unami". Glottolog 2.7. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History.
  3. Composed of /lən/-, "common, ordinary", with sense extension suggesting "original, real" + /-aːpːe/ "person"
  4. Bright, William (2004). Native American Place Names in the United States. Norman: University of Oklahoma Press, p. 251
  5. Kraft, Herbert C., The Lenape-Delaware Indian Heritage: 10,000 BC to AD 2000, Lenape Books, p.9, n.2
  6. Lenapehoking Map, showing approximate locations of Lenape Indian Bands, Lenape Lifeways website (accessed December 16, 2007)
  7. 1 2 Goddard, Ives. 1996. "Introduction." Ives Goddard, ed., The Handbook of North American Indians, Volume 17. Languages, pp. 1-16. Washington, D.C.: The Smithsonian Institution.
  8. Goddard, Ives, 1978
  9. Pentland, David, 1982
  10. Goddard, Ives. 1982. "Munsee historical phonology." International Journal of American Linguistics 48: 16-48.
  11. Goddard, Ives, 1982; Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 11,
  12. "Lenape Language Preservation Project." Delaware Tribe of Indians. Retrieved 8 Dec 2013.
  13. Heckewelder, Rev. John: History, Manners, and Customs of the Indian Nations Who Once Inhabited Pennsylvania and the Neighboring States, pages 353-432. Facsimile reprint by Heritage Books, Bowie, MD, 1990, ISBN 1-55613-411-8
  14. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. vi; for a consonant chart see Ives Goddard, 1997, pp. 43-98.
  15. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 22, xii (rule U-6); p. 26 (rule U-27)
  16. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. viii
  17. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 28
  18. Goddard, Ives, 1997: 45
  19. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 21
  20. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. xv
  21. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12, 22
  22. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 18-19
  23. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 21, 130
  24. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 12-13
  25. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 20-26
  26. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 31-34
  27. Goddard, Ives, 1979, pp. 30-31
  28. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 47
  29. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 29
  30. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 167-191
  31. 1 2 Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 76
  32. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 78
  33. Goddard, Ives, 1979, p. 73
  34. Goddard, Ives, 1979

References

Unami language test of Wikipedia at Wikimedia Incubator
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/22/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.