Sluggish cognitive tempo
Sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) or Concentration Deficit Disorder (CDD) is a descriptive term for a cluster of symptoms that some researchers believe comprise a novel psychiatric disorder. Although definitions for this proposed disorder are still being developed, some mental health professionals have characterized those affected as individuals with an attention disorder which is distinct from ADHD.[1][2]
Signs and symptoms
ADHD, the only disorder of attention currently defined by the DSM-5 or ICD-10, is subdivided into three types: a predominantly inattentive presentation, a predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation, and a combined presentation. Formal diagnosis is made by a qualified professional, and includes demonstrating six or more symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity, or both, for the three presentations, respectively. The symptoms must also be inappropriate, and must interfere with social, school, or work functioning, in addition to meeting multiple other diagnostic requirements.[3] The symptoms used in the diagnosis are shown below.[4]
ADHD symptoms | |
---|---|
Inattention | Hyperactivity-Impulsivity |
|
|
SCT is proposed to be a similar disorder to the predominantly inattentive presentation of ADHD, but, as discussed later, importantly distinct from it. One problem is that some individuals who actually have SCT are now mistakenly diagnosed with the inattentive presentation.[5] Some experts think that SCT and ADHD differ in the kind of inattention that they produce: While those with ADHD can engage their attention but fail to sustain it over time, people with SCT seem to have difficulty with engaging their attention to a specific task. Accordingly, the attentional style in SCT may be more halting and sluggish, whereas in ADHD it is jumpy and flighty.[6][7]
The list of symptoms that follows is from leading researchers of SCT, however, no universally accepted set of symptoms has been developed since this symptom cluster has not yet been recognized as an independent disorder. Additionally, requirements for a proposed diagnosis such as the number of symptoms, the duration of symptoms, and the impact on functioning are continuing to be investigated. It seems clear, however, that both ADHD and SCT are linked to impairment and a reduced quality of life.[8][9][10]
SCT symptoms (Summary) | ||
---|---|---|
|
Dysexecutive syndrome
The executive system of the human brain provides for the cross-temporal organization of behavior towards goals and the future more generally and coordinates actions and strategies for everyday goal-directed tasks. Essentially, this system permits humans to self-regulate their behavior so as to sustain action and problem solving toward goals specifically and the future more generally.[5] Dysexecutive syndrome is defined as a "cluster of impairments generally associated with damage to the frontal lobes of the brain" which includes "difficulties with high-level tasks such as planning, organising, initiating, monitoring and adapting behaviour."[11] Such executive deficits pose serious problems for a person's ability to engage in self-regulation over time to attain their goals and anticipate and prepare for the future.
Adele Diamond postulated that the core cognitive deficit of those with ADHD-PI is working memory, or, as she coined in her recent paper on the subject, "childhood-onset dysexecutive syndrome".[12] However, two more recent studies by Barkley[5] found that while children and adults with SCT had some deficits in executive functions (EF) in everyday life activities, they were primarily of far less magnitude and largely centered around problems with self-organization and problem-solving. Even then, analyses showed that most of the difficulties with EF deficits were the result of overlapping ADHD symptoms that may co-exist with SCT rather than being attributable to SCT itself. More research on the link of SCT to EF deficits is clearly indicated—but, as of this time, SCT does not seem to be as strongly associated with EF deficits as is ADHD.[5]
Causes
Unlike ADHD, the general causes of SCT symptoms are almost unknown, though one recent study of twins suggested that the condition appears to be nearly as heritable or genetically influenced in nature as ADHD.[13] That is to say that the majority of differences among individuals in these traits in the population may be due mostly to variation in their genes. The heritability of SCT symptoms in that study was only slightly lower than that for ADHD symptoms with a somewhat greater share of trait variation being due to unique environmental events. For instance, in ADHD, the genetic contribution to individual differences in ADHD traits typically averages between 75 and 80% and may even be as high as 90%+ in some studies. That for SCT may be 50-60%. Far less is known about SCT yet the symptoms seem to indicate that the posterior attention networks may be more involved in the disorder than the prefrontal cortex region of the brain and difficulties with working memory so prominent in ADHD.
This hypothesis gained greater support following a 2015 fMRI-study comparing Inattentive ADHD and SCT-disagnosed patients, which found that the SPL - Superior Parietal Lobe, showed remarkably decreased activity in SCT-cohorts.[14]
Unlike ADHD in which there exist hundreds of studies on molecular genetics identifying candidate genes that may contribute to the disorder, there are no studies of molecular genetics that specifically evaluated individuals having SCT.
Although ADHD appears to be linked to problems with the availability of or sensitivity to variations dopamine and norepinephrine, and/or the efficiency of the large chemical structures of the specific receptors and re-uptake receptors, the neurotransmitters that may be linked to SCT are unknown.
A recent study found a link between thyroid functioning and SCT symptoms, but effects were small and suggests that thyroid dysfunction is not the cause of SCT.[15] SCT symptoms were also observed in pediatric survivors of acute lymphoblastic leukemia, where they were associated with cognitive late effects.[16] Another study found high rates of SCT in children who had suffered prenatal alcohol exposure.[17]
Treatment
Treatment of SCT has not been well investigated. Initial drug studies were done only with the ADHD medication, methylphenidate (Ritalin/Concerta), and even then only with children who were diagnosed as ADD without hyperactivity (DSM-III) and not specifically for SCT. The research seems to have found that most children with DSM-III ADD-H (currently ADHD-C) responded well at medium-to-high doses.[12] However, a sizable percentage of children with ADD without hyperactivity (using DSM-III criteria; therefore the results may apply to SCT) did not gain much benefit from methylphenidate, and when they did benefit, it was at a much lower dose.[18] One study found that the presence or absence of SCT symptoms made no difference in response to methylphenidate in children with ADHD-PI.[19] Another study, a retrospective analysis of medical histories, also found that children with SCT responded well to methylphenidate.[20] But these studies did not specifically examine the effect of the drug on SCT symptoms. The only study of medication to date to specifically evaluate drug effects on SCT symptoms in children used atomoxetine (Strattera) and found it to have significant beneficial effects.[21]
Only one study has investigated the use of behavior modification methods at home and school for children with predominantly SCT symptoms and it found good success.[22]
Some SCT individuals report anecdotally that they experience improvement in their ability to focus through meditation, but this claim has not been subjected to scientific study.
In April 2014, The New York Times reported that sluggish cognitive tempo is the subject of pharmaceutical company clinical drug trials, including ones by Eli Lilly that proposed that one of its biggest-selling drugs, Strattera, could be prescribed to treat proposed symptoms of sluggish cognitive tempo.[23]
Other researchers believe that there is no effective treatment for SCT.[24]
Prognosis
The prognosis of SCT is unknown. In contrast, much is known about the adolescent and adult outcomes of children having ADHD.[25] Those with SCT symptoms typically show a later onset of their symptoms than do those with ADHD, perhaps by as much as a year or two later on average. They have as much or more difficulty with academic tasks and far fewer social difficulties than do people having ADHD (see population sample studies of SCT by Barkley, above). However, unlike ADHD, there are no longitudinal studies of children with SCT that can shed light on the developmental course and adolescent or adult outcomes of these individuals. Even so, a 2012 study by Barkley of adults[26] suggests that the disorder is present in the adult population and can be quite impairing in educational and occupational settings, even if it is not as pervasively impairing as ADHD in adults.
SCT is believed to possibly involve difficulties with selective attention or the capacity to distinguish important from unimportant information rapidly. Alternatively, as noted above, it may involve a pathological form of mind-wandering. In contrast, people with ADHD have more difficulties with persistence of attention and action toward goals coupled with impaired resistance to responding to distractions. Both disorders interfere significantly with academic performance but may do so by different means. SCT may be more problematic with the accuracy of the work a child does in school while ADHD may more adversely affect productivity, or the amount of work done in a particular time interval. Also, children with SCT may have difficulty with verbal retrieval from long term memory than children with ADHD. They also have a more disorganized thought process, a greater degree of sloppiness, and lose things more easily. They tend to have as high a risk for comorbid learning disabilities as do people with ADHD (23-50%). Yet there is some evidence that the type of learning disorders may differ in SCT, perhaps with a higher prevalence of math disorders than seen in those with ADHD. Instead of having greater difficulty selecting and filtering sensory input, as is in the case of SCT, people with ADHD have problems with inhibition.[27]
The comorbid psychiatric problems often associated with SCT are more often of the internalizing types, such as anxiety, depression, and social withdrawal.[28][29] Their typically shy nature and slow response time has often been misinterpreted as aloofness or disinterest by others. In social group interactions, those with SCT may be ignored. Those with the other types of ADHD are more likely to be rejected in social situations, because of more intrusive or aggressive behavior. Those with classic ADHD also show externalizing problems such as substance abuse, oppositional-defiant disorder, and, to a lesser degree, conduct disorder.[30][31] One study also found differential associations to personality dimensions in children: ADHD was associated with sensitivity to reward while SCT symptoms were associated with punishment sensitivity.[32]
Epidemiology
Recent studies indicate that the symptoms of SCT in children form two dimensions: daydreamy-spacey and sluggish-lethargic,[33] and that the former are more distinctive of the disorder from ADHD than the latter.[34] This same pattern was recently found in the first study of adults with SCT by Barkley.[35] This has also been the case with more recent studies of college students.[36] These studies indicated that SCT is probably not a subtype of ADHD but a distinct disorder from it. Yet it is one that overlaps with ADHD in 30-50% of cases of each disorder, suggesting a pattern of comorbidity between two related disorders rather than subtypes of the same disorder.
In many ways, those who have an SCT profile have some of the opposite symptoms of those with classic ADHD: instead of being hyperactive, extroverted, obtrusive, and risk takers, those with SCT are drifting, introspective and daydreamy, and feel as if "in the fog". Due to their drifting tendencies, those with SCT may have trouble with memory retrieval. They also do not have the same risks for oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, or social aggression,[30] and thus may have different life course outcomes compared to children with ADHD-PH and Combined subtypes who have far higher risks for these other "externalizing" disorders. A key behavioral characteristic of those with SCT symptoms is that they are more likely to appear to be lacking motivation and may even have an unusually higher frequency of daytime sleepiness.[37] They seem to lack energy to deal with mundane tasks and will consequently seek to concentrate on things that are mentally stimulating perhaps because of their underaroused state. Those with SCT symptoms may show a qualitatively different kind of attention deficit that is more typical of a true information input-output problem, such as poor focusing of attention on important details, erratic memory retrieval and active working memory, and possibly excessive mind-wandering.[38] Conversely, those with ADHD-PH and Combined subtypes are characteristically excessively energetic and have no difficulty processing information.[31]
Despite the apparent incompatibility between SCT and hyperactivity, and contrary to previous suggestions that SCT could distinguish a distinct group within the ADHD-PI type, Barkley's[35] study found that SCT was comorbid with ADHD-C almost as often as with ADHD-PI. According to a Norwegian study, "SCT correlated significantly with inattentiveness, regardless of the subtype of ADHD."[39]
Compared to individuals with ADHD-PH and Combined subtypes, children with SCT have far lower rates of comorbid oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, a higher occurrence of anxiety symptoms, and possibly a greater occurrence of depression.[40] Most consistent across studies was a pattern of social withdrawal in interactions with peers rather than the social intrusiveness, aggressiveness, and rejection so often evident in the more well-known attention disorder, ADHD. SCT is strongly correlated with ADHD inattentive and combined subtypes.[41] However, SCT can be found in individuals who would not receive an ADHD diagnosis, and it can also be found in some individuals with ADHD hyperactivity/impulsivity. Efforts are underway to encourage researchers to use the term "concentration deficit disorder" (CDD) for SCT.[30]
History
There have been many descriptions of very inattentive and foggy children in the literature. One example is The Story of Johnny Head-in-Air from Struwwelpeter. Some researchers now see the stories in this book as illustrations of many child psychiatric disorders that we know about today.[42] The Canadian pediatrician Guy Falardeau also wrote about very dreamy, quiet and well-behaved children that he encountered in his practice and called them "Les enfants lunatiques".[43]
Symptoms similar to ADHD were first systematically described in 1775 by Melchior Adam Weikard and in 1798 by Alexander Crichton in their medical textbooks. Although Weikard mainly described a single disorder of attention resembling ADHD-HI,[44] Crichton postulates an additional attention disorder, described as a "morbid diminution of its power or energy", and further explores possible "corporeal" and "mental" causes for the disorder (including "irregularities in diet, excessive evacuations, and the abuse of corporeal desires"). However, he does not further describe any symptoms of the disorder, making this an early but certainly non-specific reference to an SCT-like syndrome.[45]
In more modern times, research surrounding attention disorders has traditionally focused on hyperactive symptoms, but began to newly address inattentive symptoms in the 1970s.[46] Influenced by this research, the DSM-III (1980) allowed for the first time a diagnosis of an ADD subtype that presented without hyperactivity.[47] Researchers exploring this subtype created rating scales for children which included questions regarding symptoms such as short attention span, distractibility, drowsiness, and passivity.[46] In the mid 1980s, it was proposed that as opposed to the then accepted dichotomy of ADD with or without hyperactivity (ADD/H, ADD/noH), instead a three factor model of ADD was more appropriate, consisting of hyperactivity-impulsivity, inattention-disorganization, and slow tempo subtypes.[48]
In the 1990s, Weinberg and Brumback proposed a new disorder: "primary disorder of vigilance" (PVD). Typical symptoms of it included difficulty sustaining alertness and arousal, daydreaming, difficulty focusing attention, losing one's place in activities and conversation, slow completion of tasks and a kind personality. The most detailed case report in their article looks like a prototypical representation of SCT. The authors acknowledged an overlap of PVD and ADHD but argued in favor of considering PVD to be distinct in its unique cognitive impairments.[49][50] Problematic with the paper is that it dismissed ADHD as a nonexistent disorder (despite it having several thousand research studies by then) and preferred the term PVD for this SCT-like symptom complex. A further difficulty with the PVD diagnosis is that not only is it based merely on 6 cases instead of the far larger samples of SCT children used in other studies but the very term implies that science has established the underlying cognitive deficits giving rise to SCT symptoms, and this is hardly the case.[27]
With the publication of DSM-IV in 1994, the disorder was labeled as ADHD, and was divided into three subtypes: predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and combined.[51] Of the proposed SCT specific symptoms discussed while developing the DSM-IV, only "forgetfulness" was included in the symptom list for ADHD-I, and no others were mentioned.[46] However, several of the proposed SCT symptoms were included in the diagnosis of "ADHD, not otherwise specified".[51]
Prior to 2001, there were a total of four scientific journal articles specifically addressing symptoms of SCT.[46] In 2001, a researcher suggested that sluggish tempo symptoms were, in fact, adequate for the diagnosis of ADHD-I, and that their exclusion from DSM-IV was inappropriate.[52] The research article and its accompanying commentary urging the undertaking of more research on SCT spurred the publication of over 30 scientific journal articles to date which specifically address symptoms of SCT.[46]
However, in 2013, with the publication of DSM-V, ADHD continues to be classified as predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and combined, and there continues to be no mention of SCT as a diagnosis or a diagnosis subtype anywhere in the manual. As well, the diagnosis of "ADHD, not otherwise specified" no longer includes any mention of SCT symptoms.[3] Similarly, ICD-10, the medical diagnostic manual, has no diagnosis code for SCT.[53] Although SCT is not recognized as a disorder at this point, researchers continue to debate its usefulness as a construct and its implications for further attention disorder research.[46]
Controversy
Significant skepticism has been raised within the medical and scientific communities as to whether SCT, currently considered a "symptom cluster," actually exists as a distinct disorder.[23]
Dr. Allen Frances, an emeritus professor of psychiatry at Duke University, has commented "We're seeing a fad in evolution: Just as ADHD has been the diagnosis du jour for 15 years or so, this is the beginning of another. This is a public health experiment on millions of kids...I have no doubt there are kids who meet the criteria for this thing, but nothing is more irrelevant. The enthusiasts here are thinking of missed patients. What about the mislabeled kids who are called patients when there’s nothing wrong with them? They are not considering what is happening in the real world."[23]
UCLA researcher and Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology editorial board member Steve S. Lee has also expressed concern based on SCT's close relationship to ADHD, cautioning that a pattern of over-diagnosis of the latter has "already grown to encompass too many children with common youthful behavior, or whose problems are derived not from a neurological disorder but from inadequate sleep, a different learning disability or other sources." Lee states, "The scientist part of me says we need to pursue knowledge, but we know that people will start saying their kids have [sluggish cognitive tempo], and doctors will start diagnosing it and prescribing for it long before we know whether it’s real...ADHD has become a public health, societal question, and it’s a fair question to ask of SCT."[23]
Adding to the controversy are potential conflicts of interest among the condition's proponents, including the funding of prominent SCT researchers' work by the global pharmaceutical company Eli Lilly[23] and, in the case of Dr. Russell Barkley, a leader in the burgeoning SCT research field, direct financial ties to that company (Dr. Barkley has received $118,000 from 2009 to 2012 for consulting and speaking engagements from Eli Lilly).[54] When referring to the "increasing clinical referrals occurring now and more rapidly in the near future driven by increased awareness of the general public in SCT", Dr. Barkley writes "The fact that SCT is not recognized as yet in any official taxonomy of psychiatric disorders will not alter this circumstance given the growing presence of information on SCT at various widely visited internet sites such as YouTube and Wikipedia, among others."[55]
See also
- Cognitive Tempo
- Russell Barkley
- Pharmaceutical industry
- Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder controversies
- Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder management
- Depersonalization disorder
- Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
- Dysthymia
- Hypothyroidism
- Bipolar II disorder
- Kleine–Levin syndrome
- Low arousal theory
- Type B personality
References
- ↑ Barkley, RA (2013). "Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and adolescents: executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity.". Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 42 (2): 161–173. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.734259. Retrieved 21 April 2014.
- ↑ Barkley, RA (2012). "Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults.". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (4): 978–990. doi:10.1037/a0023961. Retrieved 21 April 2014.
- 1 2 American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. ISBN 978-0-89042-555-8.
- ↑ "Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Symptoms and Diagnosis". Division of Human Development, National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 20 February 2014. Retrieved 21 April 2014.
- 1 2 3 4 Barkley, R. A. (2012). The executive functions: What they are, how they work, and why they evolved. New York: Guilford Press.
- ↑ Ramsay, J. Russell, Anthony L. Rostain (2014). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult ADHD: An integrative psychosocial and medical approach. (2nd ed.). Routledge. pp. 11–12.
The classic presentation of ADHD involves features of high distractibility and poor attention vigilance, which can be considered as examples of attention and sustained concentration being engaged but then punctuated or interrupted. In contrast, SCT/CDD is characterized by difficulties orienting and engaging attention, effort, and alertness in the first place.
- ↑ Solanto, Mary V. (2007). "Neurocognitive Functioning in AD/HD, Predominantly Inattentive and Combined Subtypes". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 35 (5): 729–744. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9123-6.
Differences between subtypes in cognitive tempo point to potentially important differences in the qualitative features of inattention, which suggest differences in etiology. Thus, whereas children with predominantely inattentive type (PI) appear to be slow to orient and slow to respond to cognitive and social stimuli in their immediate surroundings, children with combined type (CB) rapidly orient to novel external stimuli regardless of relevance. A series of studies in children who would now be classified as CB failed to identify deficits in the stimulus input stages of information-processing (Sergeant, 2005). The observably more sluggish orientation and response style of the child with PI by contrast, does suggest deficits in these early attentional processes.
- ↑ Becker S. "The Internal, External, and Diagnostic Validity of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo: A Meta-Analysis and Critical Review (2016)". Journal of the AACAP. 55: 163–178. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006.
- ↑ Adult Concentration Inventory (ACI; Becker, Burns, & Willcutt, 2015)
- ↑ Martha A. Combs, Will H. Canu. "Impact of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms on Adults' Quality of Life". Applied Research in Quality of Life. 9: 981–995. doi:10.1007/s11482-013-9281-3.
- ↑ Wilson, Barbara A.; Alderman, Nick; Burgess, Paul W.; Emslie, Hazel; Evans, Jonathan J. (Summer 2003). "Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS)" (PDF). Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability. 5 (2): 33–37. ISSN 1740-4193.
- 1 2 Diamond A (2005). "Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): a neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (with hyperactivity)". Dev. Psychopathol. 17 (3): 807–25. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050388. PMC 1474811. PMID 16262993.
- ↑ Moruzzi S., Rijsdijk F., Battaglia M. (2014). "A Twin Study of the Relationships among Inattention, Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Problems" (PDF). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 42: 63–75. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9725-0.
- ↑ Fassbender C., Krafft C. E., Schweitzer J. B. (2015). "Differentiating SCT and inattentive symptoms in ADHD using fMRI measures of cognitive control". Neuroimage: Clinical. 8: 390–397. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.05.007.
- ↑ Becker S. P., Luebbe A. M., Greening L., Fite P. J., Stoppelbein L. (2012). "A preliminary investigation of the relation between thyroid functioning and sluggish cognitive tempo in children". Journal of Attention Disorders. doi:10.1177/1087054712466917.
- ↑ Reeves C. B., Palmer S., Gross A. M., Simonian S. J., Taylor L., Willingham E., Mulhern R. K. (2007). "Brief Report: Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Among Pediatric Survivors of Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia". Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 32 (9): 1050–1054. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm063.
- ↑ Graham D. M., Crocker N., Deweese B. N., Roesch S. C., Coles C. D., Kable J. A., Mattson S. N. (2013). "Prenatal Alcohol Exposure, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, and Sluggish Cognitive Tempo". Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 37: 338–346.
- ↑ Barkley R. A.; DuPaul G. J.; McMurray M. B. (1991). "Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: clinical response to three dose levels of methylphenidate". Pediatrics. 87 (4): 519–531.
- ↑ Ludwig H. T.; Matte B.; Katz B.; Rohde L. A. (2009). "Do Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Symptoms Predict Response to Methylphenidate in Patients with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder–Inattentive Type?". Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychopharmacology. 19 (4): 461–465. doi:10.1089/cap.2008.0115.
- ↑ Viola, L., Ruiz, R., Curone, G., Kehyaian, V., & Laxague, A. (2012). Characterization of sluggish cognitive tempo construct (SCT). Neuropsychiatrie de l’Enfance et de l’Adolescence 60(5, Supplement), S135–S136. doi:10.1016/j.neurenf.2012.04.094
- ↑ Wietecha, L., Williams, D., Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Hooper, S. R., Wigal, S. B., Dunn, D., & McBurnett, K. (2013). Atomoxetine improved attention in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia in a 16 week, acute, randomized, double-blind trial. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology Epub ahead of print.
- ↑ Pfiffner L, et al. (2007). "A Randomized, Controlled Trial of Integrated Home-School Behavioral Treatment for ADHD, Predominantly Inattentive Type". J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 46 (8): 1041–1050. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e318064675f.
- 1 2 3 4 5 Alan Schwarz (April 11, 2014). "Idea of New Attention Disorder Spurs Research, and Debate". The New York Times.
- ↑ Sluggish Cognitive Tempo a Distinct Attention Disorder? by Megan Brooks, Medscape, January 28, 2014
- ↑ Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Fischer, M. (2008). ADHD in adults: What the science says. New York: Guilford Press
- ↑ Barkley R. A. (2012). "Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (4): 978–990. doi:10.1037/a0023961.
- 1 2 Barkley, R. A. (2014). Concentration deficit disorder (sluggish cognitive tempo). In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
- ↑ Mahwah, N.J. : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, c2002- (March 2002). "Sluggish cognitive tempo predicts a different pattern of impairment in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, predominantly inattentive type". Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. ill (2002): 123–9. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3101_14. PMID 11845644.
- ↑ Becker S. P., Langberg J. M. (2012). "Sluggish cognitive tempo among young adolescents with ADHD: Relations to mental health, academic, and social functioning". Journal of Attention Disorders. 17: 681–9. doi:10.1177/1087054711435411. PMID 22441891.
- 1 2 3 Barkley R. A. (2014). "Sluggish cognitive tempo (Concentration Deficit Disorder): Current status, future directions, and a plea to change the name". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 42: 117–125. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9824-y.
- 1 2 Barkley, R. A. (2014), 4th revised edtition. Attention Deficit Hyperactivitv Disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment
- ↑ Becker Stephen P.; et al. (2013). "Reward and punishment sensitivity are differentially associated with ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in children". Journal of Research in Personality. 47: 719–727. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2013.07.001.
- ↑ Becker S. P. (2013). "Topical review: Sluggish Cognitive Tempo: Research Findings and Relevance for Pediatric Psychology". Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 38: 1051–1057. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jst058.
- ↑ Penny A. M.; Waschbusch D. A.; Klein R. M.; Corkum P.; Eskes G. (2009). "Developing a measure of sluggish cognitive tempo for children: Content validity, factor structure, and reliability". Psychological Assessment. 21 (3): 380–389. doi:10.1037/a0016600. PMID 19719349.
- 1 2 Barkley R. A. (2011). "May 23). Distinguishing Sluggish Cognitive Tempo From Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121: 978–990. doi:10.1037/a0023961.
- ↑ Becker S. P.; Langberg J. M.; Luebbe A. M.; Dvorsky M. R.; Flannery A. J. (2013). "Sluggish cognitive tempo is associated with academic functioning and internalizing symptoms in college students with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 70: 388–403. doi:10.1002/jclp.22046.
- ↑ Becker, S. P., Luebbe, A. M., & Langberg, J. M. (in press). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder dimensions and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in relation to college students' sleep functioning. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, in press.
- ↑ Adams Z. W.; Milich R.; Fillmore M. T. (2010). "A case for the return of attention-deficit disorder in DSM-5". The ADHD Report. 18 (3): 1–6. doi:10.1521/adhd.2010.18.3.1.
- ↑ Skirbekk, Benedicte; Hansen, Berit Hjelde; Oerbeck, Beate; Kristensen, Hanne (2011). "The Relationship Between Sluggish Cognitive Tempo, Subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, and Anxiety Disorders". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 39 (4): 513–525. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9488-4. ISSN 0091-0627.
- ↑ Barkley R. A. (2013). "Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and adolescents: executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity". Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 42: 161–173. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.734259.
- ↑ Willcutt Erik G.; Chhabildas Nomita; et al. "The Internal and External Validity of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo and its Relation with DSM-IV ADHD.". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 42 (1): 21–35. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9800-6.
- ↑ Stewart M. A. (1970). "Hyperactive children". Scientific American. 222: 94–98. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0470-94.
- ↑ G. Falardeau: Les enfants hyperactifs et lunatiques, Le Jour, 1997
- ↑ Barkley R. A., Peters H. (2012). "The earliest reference to ADHD in the medical literature? Melchior Adam Weikard's description of "Attention Deficit" (Mangel der Aufmerksamkeit, Attentio Volubilis)'". Journal of Attention Disorders. 16: 623–630. doi:10.1177/1087054711432309.
- ↑ Crichton, Alexander (1798). An inquiry into the nature and origin of mental derangement: Comprehending a concise system of the physiology and pathology of the human mind and a history of the passions and their effects. Volume 1. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
- 1 2 3 4 5 6 Becker SP, Marshall SA, McBurnett K (2014). "Sluggish Cognitive Tempo in Abnormal Child Psychology: An Historical Overview and Introduction to the Special Section". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 42 (1): 1–6. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9825-x. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
- ↑ American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
- ↑ Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Schaughency EA, Atkins MS, Murphy A, Hynd G (1988). "Dimensions and Types of Attention Deficit Disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 27 (3): 330–335. doi:10.1097/00004583-198805000-00011. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
- ↑ McBurnett, Keith; Pfiffner, Linda Jo (2007). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder – Concepts, Controversies, New Directions. In: Chapter 32, Sluggish Cognitive Tempo: The Promise and Problems of Measuring Syndromes in the Attention Spectrum, p. 352.
- ↑ Weinberg Warren A.; Brumback Roger A (1990). "Primary disorder of vigilance: A novel explanation of inattentiveness, daydreaming, boredom, restlessness, and sleepiness". The Journal of Pediatrics. 116 (5): 720–725. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(05)82654-X.
- 1 2 American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
- ↑ McBurnett K, Pfiffner LJ, Frick PJ (2001). "Symptom properties as a function of ADHD type: An argument for continued study of sluggish cognitive tempo". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 29 (3): 207–213. doi:10.1023/A:1010377530749. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
- ↑ World Health Organization. (2008). ICD-10: International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (10th Rev. ed.). New York, NY: Author.
- ↑ "Has Your Health Professional Received Drug Company Money?". ProPublica. Retrieved 17 April 2014.
- ↑ Barkley RA (2014). "Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (Concentration Deficit Disorder?): Current Status, Future Directions, and a Plea to Change the Name". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 42 (1): 117–125. doi:10.1007/s10802-013-9824-y. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
External links
- Free measures of SCT from leading researchers
- Cincinnati Children's Hospital: A Fuzzy Debate About A Foggy Condition
- World of Psychology: Does Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) exist?